Published 2024-11-08
Keywords
- Estados perceptivos puros,
- Contenido no conceptual,
- Formato icónico,
- Fenoménicamente peculiar
- Pure Perceptual States,
- Nonconceptual Content,
- Iconic Format,
- Phenomenally Peculiar
This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial 4.0 International License.
Abstract
The idea of pure perception —perception without any cognitive influence— is central to the science and philosophy of perception. For many, to be in a pure perceptual state is to be in a state whose content is nonconceptual, whose format is iconic, and whose phenomenology is unique. This paper explores this possibility and finds that the idea of pure perception, at least when defined in these terms, is untenable. Besides significant specific worries derived from the properties characterizing these states, I have not found empirical grounds indicating the possibility of individuating these states: neither looking inside oneself (introspection), nor looking inside others (psychology), nor looking inside the brain (neuroanatomy), suggests the existence of pure perceptual mental states.
References
- Balcetis, E., & Dunning, D. (2010). Wishful seeing: More desired objects are seen as closer. Psychological Science, 21(1), 147-152. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797609356283
- Bar, M., & Bubic, A. (2013). Top-down effects in visual perception. In K. N. Ochsner & S. Kosslyn (Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Cognitive Neuroscience, Volume 1: Core Topics (pp. 60-73). Oxford Library of Psychology. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199988693.013.0004
- Beck, J. (2012). The generality constraint and the structure of thought. Mind, 121(483), 563-600. https://doi.org/10.1093/mind/fzs077
- Beck, J. (2018). Marking the perception-cognition boundary: The criterion of stimulus-dependence. Australasian Journal of Philosophy, 96(2), 319-334. https://doi.org/10.1080/00048402.2017.1329329
- Bentin, S., Golland, Y., Flevaris, A., Robertson, L. C., & Moscovitch, M. (2006). Processing the trees and the forest during initial stages of face perception: Electrophysiological evidence. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18(8), 1406-1421. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2006.18.8.1406
- Block, N. (2014). Seeing-as in the light of vision science. Philosophy and Phenomenological Research, 89(3), 560-572. https://doi.org/10.1111/phpr.12135
- Block N. (2016). Tweaking the concepts of perception and cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 39, e232. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x15002733
- Brewer, B. (1999). Perception and reason. Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/0199250456.001.0001
- Brewer, B. (2006). Perception and content. European Journal of Philosophy, 14(2), 165-181. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-0378.2006.00220.x
- Burge, T. (2010). Origins of objectivity. Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199581405.001.0001
- Burge, T. (2014). Reply to Block: Adaptation and the upper border of perception. Philosophy and Phenomenological Research, 89(3), 573-583. https://doi.org/10.1111/phpr.12136
- Burge T. (2018). Iconic representation: Maps, pictures, and perception. In S. Wuppuluri & F. Doria (Eds), The map and the territory (pp. 79-100). The Frontiers Collection. Springer. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-72478-2_5
- Caddigan, E., Choo, H., Fei-Fei, L., & Beck, D. M. (2017). Categorization influences detection: A perceptual advantage for representative exemplars of natural scene categories. Journal of Vision, 17(1), 21, 1-11. https://doi.org/10.1167/17.1.21
- Carey, S. (2009). The origin of concepts. Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195367638.001.0001
- Carvalho, M., Justo, J., Gratier, M., & Silva, H. (2018). The impact of maternal voice on the fetus: A systematic review. Current Women´s Health Reviews, 15(1), 196-206. http://dx.doi.org/10.2174/1573404814666181026094419
- Cavedon-Taylor, D. (2021). Mental imagery: Pulling the plug on perceptualism. Philosophical Studies, 178, 3847-3868. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11098-021-01628-x
- Cermeño-Aínsa, S. (2020). The cognitive penetrability of perception: A blocked debate and a tentative solution. Consciousness and cognition, 77, 102838. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2019.102838
- Cermeño-Aínsa, S. (2021). Is perception stimulus-dependent? Review of Philosophy and Psychology, 13(3), 735-754. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1007/s13164-021-00558-1
- Crane, T. (1992). The nonconceptual content of experience. In T. Crane (Ed.), The contents of experience: Essays on perception (pp. 136-157). Cambridge University Press.
- Crouzet, S. M., Kirchner, H., & Thorpe, S. J. (2010). Fast saccades toward faces: Face detection in just 100 ms. Journal of Vision, 10(4), 16, 1-17. https://doi.org/10.1167/10.4.16
- Davidson, D. (1982). Rational animals. Dialectica, 36(4), 317-327. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1746-8361.1982.tb01546.x
- Davidson, D. (1999). The emergence of thought. Erkenntnis, 51(1), 7-17. http://www.jstor.org/stable/20012936
- de Gelder, B., & Rouw, R. (2000). Configural face processes in acquired and developmental prosopagnosia: Evidence for two separate face systems? Neuroreport, 11(14), 3145-3150. https://doi.org/10.1097/00001756-200009280-00021
- Dretske, F. (1981). Knowledge and the flow of information. The MIT Press.
- Dunning, D., & Balcetis, E. (2013). Wishful seeing: How preferences shape visual perception. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 22(1), 33-37. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721412463693
- Ellis, G. F. R. (2019). Top-down effects in the brain. Physics of Life Reviews, 31, 11-27. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.plrev.2018.05.006
- Evans, G. (1982). The varieties of reference. Oxford University Press.
- Farkas, K. (2006). Indiscriminability and the sameness of appearance. Proceedings of the Aristotelian Society, 106(1), 207-227. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9264.2006.00145.x
- Felleman, D.J., & Van Essen, D.C. (1991). Distributed hierarchical processing in the primate cerebral cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 1(1), 1-47. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/1.1.1-a
- Firestone, C., & Scholl, B. J. (2014). “Top-down” effects where none should be found: The El Greco fallacy in perception research. Psychological Science, 25(1), 38-46. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797613485092
- Firestone, C., & Scholl, B. J. (2016). Cognition does not affect perception: Evaluating the evidence for ‘top-down’ effects. Behavioural and Brain Sciences, 39, 1-77. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x15000965
- Fish, W. J. (2008). Disjunctivism, indistinguishability and the nature of hallucination. In A. Haddock & F. Macpherson (Eds.), Disjunctivism: Perception, action, and knowledge (pp. 144-167). Oxford University Press.
- Fodor, J. A. (1983). The modularity of mind. The MIT Press.
- Fodor, J. A. (2007). The revenge of the given. In B. McLaughlin & J. Cohen (Eds.), Contemporary debates in philosophy of mind (pp. 105-116). Blackwell.
- Gervain, J. (2018). The role of prenatal experience in language development. Current Opinion in Behavioural Science, 21, 62-67. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2018.02.004
- Gilbert, C. D., & Li, W. (2013). Top-down influences on visual processing. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 14, 350-363. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn3476
- Glock, H. J. (2000). Animals, thoughts, and concepts. Synthese, 123, 35-64. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1005295521736
- Green, E. J., & Quilty-Dunn, J. (2021). What is an object file? The British Journal for the Philosophy of Science, 72(3), 665-699. https://doi.org/10.1093/bjps/axx055
- Grill-Spector, K., & Kanwisher, N. (2005). Visual recognition: As soon as you know it is there, you know what it is. Psychological Science, 16(2), 152-160. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.00796.x
- Groen, I. I. A., Silson, E. H., & Baker, C. I. (2017). Contributions of low- and high-level properties to neural processing of visual scenes in the human brain. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, Biological Sciences, 372, 20160102. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2016.0102
- Hansen, T., Olkkonen, M., Walter, S., & Gegenfurtner, K. R. (2006). Memory modulates color appearance. Nature Neuroscience, 9(11), 1367-1368. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1794
- Hanson, R. H. (1958). Observation and explanation: A guide to philosophy of science. Cambridge University Press.
- Harel, A., Groen, I. I., Kravitz, D. J., Deouell, L. Y., & Baker, C. I. (2016). The temporal dynamics of scene processing: A multifaceted EEG investigation. eNeuro, 3(5). https://doi.org/10.1523/ENEURO.0139-16.2016
- Harnad, S. (2005). To cognize is to categorize: Cognition is categorization. In C. Lefebvre & H. Cohen (Eds.), Handbook of categorization in cognitive science (pp. 20-43). Elsevier.
- Hume, D. (1967). A treatise of human nature. Oxford University Press.
- Jastrzębowska, M. A., Chicherov, V., Draganski, B., & Herzog, M. H. (2021). Unraveling brain interactions in vision: The example of crowding. NeuroImage, 240, 118390. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2021.118390
- Kirchner, H., & Thorpe, S. J. (2006). Ultra-rapid object detection with saccadic eye movements: Visual processing speed revisited. Vision Research, 46(11), 1762-1776. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2005.10.002
- Kosslyn, S. (2005). Mental images and the brain. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 22(3/4), 333-347. https://doi.org/10.1080/02643290442000130
- Kuhn, T. S. (1962). The structure of scientific revolutions. University of Chicago Press.
- Levin, D. T., & Banaji, M. R. (2006). Distortions in the perceived lightness of faces: The role of race categories. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 135(4), 501-512. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.135.4.501
- Lewis, M. B., & Edmonds, A. J. (2003). Face detection: Mapping human performance. Perception, 32(8), 903-920. https://doi.org/10.1068/p5007
- Löhr, G. (2020). Concepts and categorization: Do philosophers and psychologists theorize about different things? Synthese, 197, 2171-2191. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11229-018-1798-4
- Lupyan, G. (2015). Cognitive penetrability of perception in the age of prediction: Predictive systems are penetrable systems. Review of Philosophy and Psychology, 6(4), 547-569. https://doi.org/10.1007/s13164-015-0253-4
- Maguire, J. F., & Howe, P. D. L. (2016). Failure to detect meaning in RSVP at 27 ms per picture. Attention Perception and Psychophysics, 78(5), 1405-1413. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-016-1096-5
- Mandelbaum, E. (2018). Seeing and conceptualizing: Modularity and the shallow contents of perception. Philosophy and Phenomenological Research, 97(2), 267-283. https://doi.org/10.1111/phpr.12368
- Markov, N. T., Ercsey-Ravasz, M., Van Essen, D. C., Knoblauch, K., Toroczkai, Z., & Kennedy, H. (2013). Cortical high-density counter stream architectures. Science, 342, 1238406. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1238406
- Martin, M. G. F. (2002). The transparency of experience. Mind and Language, 17(4), 376-425. https://doi.org/10.1111/1468-0017.00205
- Masrour, F., Nirshberg, G., Schon, M., Leardi, J., & Barrett, E. (2015). Revisiting the empirical case against perceptual modularity. Frontiers in psychology, 6, 1676. https://doi.org/10.3389%2Ffpsyg.2015.01676
- McDowell, J. (1994). Mind and world. Harvard University Press.
- Michotte, A. (1963). The perception of causality. Basic Books.
- Nanay, B. (2015). Perceptual content and the content of mental imagery. Philosophical Studies: An International Journal for Philosophy in the Analytic Tradition, 172(7), 1723-1736. http://www.jstor.org/stable/24704178
- Newen, A., & Bartels, A. (2007). Animal minds and the possession of concepts. Philosophical Psychology, 20(3), 283-308. https://doi.org/10.1080/09515080701358096
- O’Callaghan, C., Kveraga, K., Shine, J. M., Adams, R. B., & Bar, M. (2016). Convergent evidence for top-down effects from the “predictive brain”. Behavioural and Brain Sciences, 39, e254. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X15002599
- Otsuka, Y. (2014). Face recognition in infants: A review of behavioural and near-infrared stereoscopic studies. Japanese Psychological Research, 56(1), 76-90. https://doi.org/10.1111/jpr.12024
- Peacocke, C. (1992). A study of concepts. The MIT Press.
- Peacocke, C. (2001). Does perception have a nonconceptual content? Journal of Philosophy, 98(5), 239-264. https://doi.org/10.2307/2678383
- Pearson, J., Naselaris, T., Holmes, E. A., & Kosslyn, S. M. (2015). Mental imagery: Functional mechanisms and clinical applications. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 19(1), 590-602. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2015.08.003
- Perky, C. W. (1910). An experimental study of imagination. The American Journal of Psychology, 21(3), 422-452. https://doi.org/10.2307/1413350
- Peters, A. (2002). Examining neocortical circuits: Some background and facts. Journal of Neurocytology, 31, 183-193. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1024157522651
- Phillips, B. (2019). The shifting border between perception and cognition. Noûs, 53(2), 316-346. https://doi.org/10.1111/nous.12218
- Pitt, D. (2004). The phenomenology of cognition: Or what is it like to think that P? Philosophy and Phenomenological Research, 69(1), 1-36. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1933-1592.2004.tb00382.x
- Potter, M. C., Wyble, B., Hagmann, C. E., & McCourt, E. (2014). Detecting meaning in RSVP at 13 ms per picture. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 76(2), 270-279. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-013-0605-z
- Provasi, J., Anderson, D. I., & Barbu-Roth, M. (2014). Rhythm perception, production, and synchronization during the perinatal period. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1048. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01048
- Pylyshyn, Z. W. (1999). Is vision continuous with cognition? The case for cognitive impenetrability of visual perception. Behavioural and Brain Sciences, 22(3), 341-365. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x99002022
- Quilty-Dunn, J. (2016). Iconicity and the format of perception. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 23(3/4), 255-263.
- Quilty-Dunn, J. (2017). Syntax and semantics of perceptual representation. Ph.D. dissertation. CUNY Academic Works.
- https://academicworks.cuny.edu/gc_etds/2261
- Quilty-Dunn, J. (2020). Perceptual pluralism. Noûs, 54(4), 807-838. https://doi.org/10.1111/nous.12285
- Ramkumar, P., Hansen, B. C., Pannasch, S., & Loschky, L. C. (2016). Visual information representation and rapid scene categorization are simultaneous across cortex: An MEG study. NeuroImage, 134, 295-304. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.03.027
- Rolfs, M., Dambacher, M., & Cavanagh, P. (2013). Visual adaptation of the perception of causality. Current Biology, 23(3), 250-254. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2012.12.017
- Rolfs, M., & Dambacher, M. (2016). What draws the line between perception and cognition? The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 39, e257. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x15002617
- Schellenberg, S. (2011). Perceptual content defended. Noûs, 45(4), 714-750. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-0068.2010.00791.x
- Scholl, B. J., & Gao, T. (2013). Perceiving animacy and intentionality: Visual processing or higher-level judgment? In M. D. Rutherford & V. A. Kuhlmeier (Eds.), Social perception: Detection and interpretation of animacy, agency, and intention (pp. 197-229). The MIT Press.
- Seyfarth, R. M., & Cheney, D. L. (2015). The evolution of concepts about agents: Or, what do animals recognize when they recognize an individual? In E. Margolis & S. Laurence (Eds.), The conceptual mind: New directions in the study of concepts (pp. 57-76). The MIT Press.
- Siegel, S. (2004). Indiscriminability and the phenomenal. Philosophical Studies, 120, 90-112. https://doi.org/10.1023/B:PHIL.0000033752.70521.13
- Siegel, S. (2008). The epistemic conception of hallucination. In A. Haddock & F. Macpherson (Eds.), Disjunctivism: Perception, action, and knowledge (pp. 205-224). Oxford University Press.
- Soteriou, M. (2020). The disjunctive theory of perception. In Ed. N. Zalta (Ed.), The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy (Summer 2020 Edition). https://plato.stanford.edu/archives/sum2020/entries/perception-disjunctive/
- Speaks, J., (2005). Is there a problem about nonconceptual content? Philosophical Review, 114(3), 359-398. https://doi.org/10.1215/00318108-114-3-359
- Sturgeon, S. (2006). Reflective disjunctivism. Supplement to the Proceedings of the Aristotelian Society, 80(1), 185-216. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8349.2006.00142.x
- Uyar, F., Shomstein, S., Greenberg, A. S., & Behrmann, M. (2016). Retinotopic information interacts with category selectivity in human ventral cortex. Neuropsychologia 92, 90-106. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.05.022
- Valenti, J. J., & Firestone, C. (2019). Finding the “odd one out”: Memory color effects and the logic of appearance. Cognition, 191, 103934. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2019.04.003
- Veniero, D., Gross, J., Morand, S., Duecker, F., Sack, A. T., & Thut, G. (2021). Top-down control of visual cortex by the frontal eye fields through oscillatory realignment. Nature Communications, 12, 1757. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-021-21979-7
- Vetter, P., & Newen, A. (2014). Varieties of cognitive penetration in visual perception. Consciousness and Cognition, 27, 62-75. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2014.04.007
- Webster, M. A., & MacLeod, D. I. (2011). Visual adaptation and face perception. Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society B, Biological sciences, 366, 1702-1725. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2010.0360